Free Republic
Browse · Search
News/Activism
Topics · Post Article

Skip to comments.

How Females Choose Their Mates
Scientific American ^ | March 2002 | by Lee Alan Dugatkin and Jean-Guy J. Godin

Posted on 03/13/2002 4:44:42 PM PST by vannrox

How Females Choose Their Mates


Females often prefer to mate with the most flamboyant males. Their choice may be based on a complex interaction between instinct and imitation



by Lee Alan Dugatkin and Jean-Guy J. Godin




Picture a man who has a way with the ladies, and a character not unlike James Bond may spring to mind. He's clever, classy, fearless and flashy-characteristics that are almost universally appealing to the opposite sex. Throw in the powerful sports car, and you have a nearly irresistible combination.


That females often flock to the most ostentatious males is not a phenomenon unique to humans. In many different species, successful males--those that sire the most offspring--are often larger or more brightly colored or "show off" with more vigorous courtship displays.


Females tend to be the choosier sex when it comes to selecting a mate, partly because males can produce millions of sperm, whereas females' eggs are few and far between. Thus, females may be more selective because they have more invested in each gamete and in the resulting offspring. And because the availability of eggs is a limiting factor in reproductive success, males tend to compete for female attention and not vice versa.


GUPPY BEHAVIOR




Charles Darwin was the first to propose that competition for mates plays an important role in reproductive success--a process he dubbed sexual selection. In The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex, published in 1871, Darwin hypothesized that any trait that gives a male mating and fertilization advantages will evolve in a population because males with such traits will produce more offspring than their competitors. Assuming the trait is heritable, offspring expressing the beneficial trait will, in turn, achieve greater reproductive success than their competitors, and so on, through future generations. Further, Darwin proposed that some of these traits may have evolved because they attract the attention of females. The idea that females are discriminating and can actively choose with whom to mate was controversial from its inception--perhaps because male-male battles can be quite spectacular. Males may fight amongst themselves, occasionally in dramatic battles to the death, to gain mating privileges with females. In comparison, female choice is generally much more subtle.


Finding Mr. Right




Over the past 25 years, a considerable body of scientific evidence in support of female choice has accumulated. Females actively choose their mates in a large variety of species--particularly ones in which males are less aggressive and display individual differences in secondary sexual characteristics, such as ornamental plumage or courtship displays. Nevertheless, how and why females select their partners and how mating preferences have evolved remain hotly debated issues among evolutionary biologists.


A choosy female faces two general tasks in selecting a mate. First, she must search for and locate a male. This task can be difficult if the population is sparse or if the danger of predators prevents her from spending a good deal of time searching for a suitable mate. Once she has encountered a male, the female must then decide whether to accept or reject him as a mate. The decision often involves some shopping around. In certain mating systems, females may encounter a group of available males and can compare them on the spot. For example, in early spring, male sage grouse (Centrocercus urophasianus) aggregate "cheek-to-jowl" in temporary communal mating arenas called leks, where they strut their stuff for the females. A female typically observes the displays of a number of males, apparently comparing them before mating with one lucky suitor. She then leaves the lek to nest and raise her brood elsewhere. Of all the potential mates on a lek, a few preferred males receive the bulk of the female attention.


But males are not always conveniently displayed like chocolates in a sampler box. More commonly, females encounter males one at a time. Comparing males in this case is presumably a more challenging cognitive task, as it involves remembering the characteristics of an individual that is no longer in sight. Studies have shown that females can rank the characteristics of sequentially presented males.


Theo C. M. Bakker and Manfred Milinski of the University of Bern in Switzerland found that female three-spined sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus) will tailor their mate choice to the relative attractiveness of the present and previously encountered males. Females were more likely to show interest in a male if his red nuptial coloring was brighter than the previous male's and more likely to reject a suitor whose coloring was less bright than his predecessor's.


Whether a female chooses her mate from among a dozen dancing grouse or between a pair of crimson fish, she generally selects the most conspicuous contender. Empirical evidence indicates that females commonly prefer male traits that most strongly stimulate their senses. (This evidence has recently been reviewed by Malte Andersson of the University of Göteborg in Sweden and by Michael J. Ryan of the University of Texas at Austin and Anne C. Keddy-Hector of Austin Community College.) For example, when given a choice, female green tree frogs (Hyla cinerea) are preferentially attracted to males that call the loudest and most frequently; female guppies (Poecilia reticulata) to the most brightly colored males; and female mallards (Anas platyrhynchos) to males that court them most frequently. Because of such preferences, males have typically evolved exaggerated secondary sexual traits to attract the opposite sex.


Why Be Choosy?




Even though evidence indicates that females can actively choose their mates, the question of why females discriminate, rather than mate at random, remains largely unresolved. How did female choice originate and evolve? What are its benefits and costs to individual females?


In some cases, females may favor mating with a male that is loud or brightly colored simply because he is easy to locate. Reducing the amount of time it takes to find a mate may reduce a female's risk of being killed by a predator. But for many species, mate choice is probably more complex. For many birds and mammals, natural selection appears to favor females who choose mates that provide them with some direct benefit that will increase their fecundity, their survival or the survival of their offspring. Such benefits might include food, a safe haven or even the prospect of fewer parasites.


In a long-term study of the barn swallow (Hirundo rustica), Anders P. Møller of the CNRS in Paris observed that females prefer to mate with males possessing elongated tail feathers. As it turns out, the long-tailed males are infected with fewer bloodsucking mites than their short-tailed counterparts. Because these parasites can jump from bird to bird, females that mate with long-tailed males benefit by avoiding infection and by producing greater numbers of healthier chicks than females that mate with shorter-tailed males. Unfortunately, because selecting a mate that offers direct benefits seems so obvious, few studies have tested this evolutionary model in a rigorous way.


When males provide no obvious resources, such as food or protection, females may choose to mate with the males that appear to have the best genes. How do they know which males have good genes? And why don't males just cheat by faking the traits associated with such genes? In 1975 Amotz Zahavi of the University of Tel Aviv in Israel suggested that females assess only those traits that are honest indicators of male fitness--a hypothesis known as the handicap principle. Honest indicators, which are "costly" to produce and maintain, should be associated with the most vigorous males.


While studying antipredator behavior in the Trinidadian guppy, we recently obtained some evidence that is consistent with the handicap principle. When a predatory fish nears a school of guppies, males, often in pairs, cautiously approach the potential threat to "inspect" it. Such risky behavior has been observed in many species, and behavioral ecologists have suggested that bold males may swim close to a predator to advertise their vigor to nearby females. In fact, laboratory studies have shown that when no females are present, no male guppy plays the hero by approaching the predator more often than his counterpart.


We hypothesized that boldness exhibited during predator inspection might be attractive to females because it should be a reliable indicator of fitness. Less vigorous guppies who tried to "fake" competence in predator inspection would likely be eaten. By using small, custom-built containers that allowed us to position males at different distances from a predator fish, we found that females indeed preferred the most intrepid males. Such courage appears to correlate with color: the males that swim closest to the predator are usually the most colorful. Thus, in the wild, females may have evolved a preference for the flashier males because color is a proxy for boldness and fitness.


Once females have expressed a preference for a certain trait, a process called runaway selection can occur. The model, first brought to the attention of evolutionary biologists by Ronald Fisher in 1958, suggests that a male trait and the female preference for that trait coevolve. For example, females that prefer to mate with large males should produce large sons as well as daughters that show a preference for large males. Under certain conditions, this process can escalate, producing increasingly exaggerated male traits and stronger female preference for those traits.


A number of behavioral ecologists have found some evidence for runaway coevolution of orange body coloration in male guppies and of female preference for this male trait. But a more convincing example of runaway selection has recently been presented by Gerald S. Wilkinson and Paul Reillo of the University of Maryland in their study of the stalk-eyed fly (Cyrtodiopsis dalmanni). In this species, females generally prefer to mate with males possessing widely spaced eyes. By selectively breeding the flies for 13 generations, Wilkinson and Reillo generated one line of flies in which the males had large eyestalks and another line of shorter-stalked males. They found that females in each line preferred the male trait selected for in that line--that is, females from the large-stalk line preferred males with the longest stalks, and females from the short-stalk line preferred shorter-stalked males. Female preference thus coevolved with the selected male trait.


How do preferences about mate choice originate? In some cases, females may have a preexisting sensory bias for a certain trait, not because it represents anything but because it attracts attention--a hypothesis championed most prominently by Ryan and John Endler of James Cook University in Australia. For example, female swordtails (Xiphophorus helleri) prefer males with long "swords" on their tail fins. And although males of a related species--the platyfish Xiphophorus maculatus-lack swords completely, Alexandra L. Basolo of the University of Nebraska found that when she attached artificial, plastic swords onto these naturally swordless males, female platyfish showed an immediate, strong and consistent preference for the males with the counterfeit swords. In other words, platyfish females harbored a preexisting bias for long swords, even though swords reveal nothing about the fitness of platyfish males.


These evolutionary models may be operating separately or in conjunction; it is difficult to untangle them experimentally. Female guppies, for instance, may be partial to orange males because bright coloring is a proxy for boldness or for good health (males with the brightest pigments are probably eating well). But the preference could have originated because females are more attuned to colors of a particular wavelength and then further evolved through a runaway mechanism.


All these models assume that female preference is genetically determined. Recent studies indicate, however, that social factors, such as imitation, also influence mate choice.


Copycat Birds and Fish




Some guys get all the girls. On a crowded grouse lek, for example, the top male may receive 80 percent of the mating opportunities. Is he simply irresistible? Or do females take one another's choices into account when selecting a mate? In the early 1990s a group of Scandinavian researchers, led by Jacob Höglund and Arne Lundberg of Uppsala University and Rauno Alatalo of Jyväskylä University, initiated a detailed study of mate-choice copying in the black grouse (Tetrao tetrix). Using stuffed dummies to represent interested females, the researchers showed that female grouse mated preferentially with the male that appeared to have other females in his territory.


Why copy? Perhaps imitation teaches females what to look for in a male. In an extensive series of experiments on mate-choice copying in guppies, we determined that young females are more likely to copy the mate choice of older, more experienced females than vice versa. Further, copying may save time. Relying on the judgment of others may allow a female to assess a potential mate quickly and efficiently, leaving her more time to forage for food or hide from predators.


RUNAWAY SELECTION




For species in which females copy, a fascinating question emerges: How much of female mate choice is based on instinct and how much on imitation? To tease apart the relative contributions of genetic and social factors involved in mate choice in guppies from the Paria River in Trinidad, one of us (Dugatkin) carried out a behavioral "titration" experiment. First, a female guppy was allowed to choose between two males that differed in the amount of orange that covered their bodies. As expected, females virtually always chose the more orange of a pair of males. Then a copying opportunity was staged, in which the test female was allowed to observe another female apparently choosing the less orange male as her putative mate.


Which male did she then choose for herself? Remember that the female's genetic predisposition is "pulling" her toward the more orange male, but social cues and the potential to copy are tugging her toward the drabber male. In the end, her choice depended on how much the males differed in coloration. When the paired males differed by small (12 percent) or moderate (25 percent) amounts of orange, the female consistently chose the less orange of the two. In this case, the female succumbed to peer pressure, her tendency to copy overriding her genetic preference for orange males. If, however, the males differed by a large amount (40 percent) of orange, the female ignored the seemingly bad advice and chose the more orange male, her genetic predisposition masking any copying effects.


It appears as if there exists in guppies a color threshold, below which social cues steer female mate choice and above which genetic factors predominate. Dugatkin is performing further experiments to assess whether copying behavior in guppies is itself heritable. Although imitation appears to be based on social cues, perhaps genes govern the likelihood that a female guppy will engage in copying behavior.


Sadie Hawkins Day




Although people are more complex than guppies and grouse, some of the same mate-choice rules may apply to human dating games. According to popular wisdom, it is human females who are the choosier sex when it comes to selecting a mate. As a species, humans meet the criteria for female choice: men, for the most part, will avoid fighting to the death for the hand of a young maiden. And females can distinguish between various males on the basis of differences in their characteristics: some men are brasher, some are brighter and some have bigger bank accounts.


Women may even engage in mate-choice copying. After all, imitation is important in many types of human learning. To determine whether copying plays a role in how women rate a man's attractiveness, Dugatkin is currently collaborating with social psychologists Michael Cunningham and Duane Lundy of theUniversity of Louisville. Although their results are preliminary, they find that women are more likely to express an interest in going out with a man if they are told that other women also find him attractive.


Of course, evolutionary theory will never be able to explain fully singles bars, personal ads or cyber-romance. Even for animals, it appears that the benefits and costs of being choosy when selecting a mate differ for different species, in different environments and sometimes at different times of day. In any case, if animals as simple as guppies can consider the opinions of their peers when choosing a mate, imagine how complex the cues must be that guide humans in their search for the perfect mate.




TOPICS: Culture/Society; Extended News; Front Page News; Miscellaneous; News/Current Events
KEYWORDS: attraction; crevolist; man; marrage; mates; race; sex; women
Navigation: use the links below to view more comments.
first previous 1-20 ... 201-220221-240241-260 ... 301-313 next last
To: weikel
Oh you'll be horny forever but either you won't want to be a lecher or you won't want to cheat or you'll need the little blue triangles...LOL
221 posted on 03/13/2002 8:21:27 PM PST by wardaddy
[ Post Reply | Private Reply | To 205 | View Replies]

To: mackattack
Flame Warriors

Cyber Sisters

Cyber Sisters are an extremely fierce confederation of fighting females who act something like a shrill Greek chorus, echoing and amplifying one another's voice until their foes retreat in disarray. They are generally leaderless, but anyone who challenges one Cyber Sister can expect to be savagely attacked by the others. Only the most powerful and battle-hardened of Warriors is strong enough to weather a Cyber Sisters attack.

Im your man mack.

222 posted on 03/13/2002 8:21:53 PM PST by weikel
[ Post Reply | Private Reply | To 215 | View Replies]

To: mackattack
Fair enough. Just don't get the ugly dudes fired because they hit on you. Geeeeez!
223 posted on 03/13/2002 8:22:16 PM PST by Tolerance Sucks Rocks
[ Post Reply | Private Reply | To 177 | View Replies]

To: Chewbacca
I prefer the old, rusty pickup truck look.
Keeps the gold diggers away and me from getting robbed or car jacked.

I like yer style. Got any sisters?

224 posted on 03/13/2002 8:22:24 PM PST by meyer
[ Post Reply | Private Reply | To 209 | View Replies]

To: Dan from Michigan
Exactly.
225 posted on 03/13/2002 8:22:28 PM PST by weikel
[ Post Reply | Private Reply | To 216 | View Replies]

To: weikel
All feminazis should be required to watch the Sexual Harassment Panda episode.
226 posted on 03/13/2002 8:23:30 PM PST by Tolerance Sucks Rocks
[ Post Reply | Private Reply | To 184 | View Replies]

To: mackattack
TY!!!...this will be the last unless God has other plans...we'll take whatever bounty comes our way.
227 posted on 03/13/2002 8:23:36 PM PST by wardaddy
[ Post Reply | Private Reply | To 210 | View Replies]

To: mackattack
I will ping you, but you will be busy. I havent learned my place yet, so all the old maids pick on me. :-(

Give me a couple of months to get my head on straight again and I will tell you to just refer the old maids to me. By the way with the astroturf line I left the raunchier lines out but I am certain you and everyone else can come up with them.

Stay well - Stay safe - Stay armed - Yorktown

228 posted on 03/13/2002 8:23:41 PM PST by harpseal
[ Post Reply | Private Reply | To 215 | View Replies]

To: Tolerance Sucks Rocks
They wouldn't understand it they would just kill Trey and Matt all feminazis should just be killed along with all other militant liberal commies( they want to do it to us).
229 posted on 03/13/2002 8:25:30 PM PST by weikel
[ Post Reply | Private Reply | To 226 | View Replies]

To: Tolerance Sucks Rocks
Jujitsu.... I tried it once and liked it, but I had doubts as to how effective it could be against someone stronger then I was/am.
230 posted on 03/13/2002 8:26:06 PM PST by Jason Kauppinen
[ Post Reply | Private Reply | To 220 | View Replies]

To: weikel; mackattack
Well, there is that...:-(
231 posted on 03/13/2002 8:26:29 PM PST by Tolerance Sucks Rocks
[ Post Reply | Private Reply | To 198 | View Replies]

Comment #232 Removed by Moderator

Comment #233 Removed by Moderator

To: weikel; mackattack
I will, too! You honesty hurts, but it's a useful and, in the long run, pleasurable sort of pain. :-)
234 posted on 03/13/2002 8:28:44 PM PST by Tolerance Sucks Rocks
[ Post Reply | Private Reply | To 204 | View Replies]

To: mackattack
I don't know what the guild is like I said I don't start trouble I just... finish it. I can beat them without doing things that will get me banned( I doubt that would get me banned). Just ping me when they are flaming you IF I AM ON I will come to your rescue right away if not then I will start flaming your attackers next time im on FR.
235 posted on 03/13/2002 8:29:34 PM PST by weikel
[ Post Reply | Private Reply | To 232 | View Replies]

Comment #236 Removed by Moderator

To: Jason Kauppinen
Like I said, go for a combo, "bastardized" coarse, and forget Marquis de Queensbury when you're in a bar or out on the street. He's a jerk, you're right, he's wrong! Just remember, if he has weapons (or, I guess, if he's a LOT bigger than you), it's best to run if you can (sigh).
237 posted on 03/13/2002 8:33:39 PM PST by Tolerance Sucks Rocks
[ Post Reply | Private Reply | To 230 | View Replies]

To: Lithasis
LOL.... or lick the bottom of a pint glass..... from the inside of the glass.
238 posted on 03/13/2002 8:33:40 PM PST by Jason Kauppinen
[ Post Reply | Private Reply | To 236 | View Replies]

To: Jason Kauppinen
Your size and build reminds me of a member of UDT-14 I once knew. He was that way into BUD/S and even after he came out he did not look like he was well muscled but anyone who started something with him was in for severe trouble. Take a variety of martial arts course if for no other reason than the self control necessary to master any of the disciplines will provide a great deal of confidence. There is nobody who is invincible there are only men who are willing to put it all on the line and who are will to train to do the best possible job when forced into a fight.

In short you do not have to be the baddest dude on the planet just more willing to try than the other guy. A comprehensive approach to self defense is what will work.

In the final analysis that is the basic presumption behind CCW the person who carries a sidearm for self-defense will usually try to walk away from most fights and willonly fight when cornered and there is no other choice but when faced with that situation will survive by resorting to overwhelming force.

Stay well - Stay safe- Stay armed - Yorktown

239 posted on 03/13/2002 8:35:18 PM PST by harpseal
[ Post Reply | Private Reply | To 230 | View Replies]

To: Tolerance Sucks Rocks
*nods* And split if I'm alone and he has friends.
240 posted on 03/13/2002 8:35:37 PM PST by Jason Kauppinen
[ Post Reply | Private Reply | To 237 | View Replies]


Navigation: use the links below to view more comments.
first previous 1-20 ... 201-220221-240241-260 ... 301-313 next last

Disclaimer: Opinions posted on Free Republic are those of the individual posters and do not necessarily represent the opinion of Free Republic or its management. All materials posted herein are protected by copyright law and the exemption for fair use of copyrighted works.

Free Republic
Browse · Search
News/Activism
Topics · Post Article

FreeRepublic, LLC, PO BOX 9771, FRESNO, CA 93794
FreeRepublic.com is powered by software copyright 2000-2008 John Robinson